THE EXPERIENCE OF READING
Alan Tonnies Moore[1]
San Francisco State University
Eric Schwitzgebel
University of California, Riverside
Abstract
What do people consciously experience when they read? There has
been almost no rigorous research on this question, and opinions diverge
radically among both philosophers and psychologists. We describe three studies
of the phenomenology of reading and its relationship to memory of textual
detail and general cognitive abilities. We find three main results. First,
there is substantial variability in reports about reading experience, both
within and between participants. Second, reported reading experience varies
with passage type: passages with dialogue prompted increased reports of inner speech,
while passages with vivid visual detail prompted increased reports of visual
imagery. Third, reports of visual imagery experiences, inner speech
experiences, and experiences of conscious visual perception of the words on the
page were at best weakly related to general cognitive abilities and memory of
visual and auditory details.
Keywords: reading, inner speech, visual imagery,
introspection, experimental aesthetics, experience sampling
1.
Introduction.
What sorts of conscious experiences
do you have while reading? You are, in fact, reading at this very moment. So
think, what are you experiencing right now?
Systematic studies that explicitly
focus on people’s self-reported conscious experiences while reading are rare. In
a way, this is surprising. Academics spend much of their lives reading. People studying
the aesthetics of fiction and poetry are interested in the reading experience (Carroll,
2001; Fish, 1970; Holland, 1975; Kivy, 2008; Lamarque & Olson, 2004; Miall
& Kuiken, 2002; Phelan, 2007; Robinson, 2005). However, almost all existing
explicit claims about people’s conscious experiences (or phenomenology) while reading
are based on unsystematic armchair introspection by the scholar in question or,
in some cases, on the retrospective reports of casual readers. This has led to
a bewildering array of assertions about the phenomenology of reading by
psychologists, philosophers, and literary theorists.
For example, some scholars assert
that, at least for them, reading a narrative, such as a story or a novel, normally
involves experiences of visual imagery
(e.g., Ahsen, 1984; Dennett, 1991, p. 366; Nannicelli, 2013; Wittgenstein,
1946-1948/1975, p. 44). Others express skepticism about the frequency of conscious
visual imagery or its empirical relation to textual details (e.g. Berkeley, 1710/2009, Intro, Section 20; Burke
1757/1990, p. 152; Kurby
& Zacks, 2013). Similarly, some scholars assert
that the phenomenology of reading normally involves inner speech, inner hearing, or some sort of voice in the head (Baars,
2003; James, 1890, p. 361; Kivy, 2008; Morin, 2009; Perrone-Bertolotti et al.,
2014; Velmans, 2009), while others deny that reading must involve such a voice
(Brouwers et al.,
2017; Faw, 2009; Reed, 1916; Woodworth, 1906, p. 704).
Scholars also disagree about whether people normally have conscious visual experience of the words on the page when they
read. Julian Jaynes (1976, p. 26), for example, says that once readers are
absorbed in the text, they have no visual experience at all of the words on the
page, that one normally experiences only the meaning of the words and has no
conscious experience of the letters as they appear on the page. Russell T.
Hurlburt (Hurlburt & Schwitzgebel 2007, p. 50) seems to agree that this is
often the case, while Charles Siewert finds this “just about as obviously false
a remark as one could make about visual experience” (1998, p. 249).
The experimental literature on the cognitive architecture involved in
reading, while large, is mostly silent on the question of readers’ conscious
experiences. Some of the most well understood cognitive processes involved in
reading are those that process phonological (sound) information. While the body
of research on phonological coding (e.g. Leinenger, 2014; Seidenberg &
McClelland, 1989; Van Orden, Johnston, & Hale, 1988) and the phonological
loop (Baddeley 2010; Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001;
Frost, 1988; Lauro, Reis, Cohen, Cecchetto, & Papagno, 2010; Paap &
Noel, 1991) show that the sound of a word plays a foundational role in processing
textual information, this process could be, and likely often is, unconscious.
Many of the cognitive processes involved in reading are largely unconscious, such
as visuospatial memory (Baddeley, 2000, 2007; Pham & Hasson, 2014) and the
use of situational models in narrative comprehension (Kurby & Zacks, 2013; Zwaan,
2016; Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998). Our eyes move multiple times a second while
reading, but this does not entail that we are conscious of each saccade, and similarly,
attunement to the sound of the words while reading does not entail an auditory experience
of any sort. Unfortunately, the experimental literature often fails
appropriately to highlight the important distinction between possibly
non-conscious cognitive processes involving phonological information and the
conscious experience of inner speech (e.g. Kurby, Magliano, & Rapp, 2009; Leinenger,
2014). The same general trend is at work in the research on the visuospatial
sketchpad and situational models of narrative comprehension. This leaves us in
a rather odd position: the experimental literature on reading has told us a lot
about the cognitive architecture recruited for reading but very little about
the conscious experiences that people have while reading.
We see two possible explanations for
the broad disagreement in reports about the experience of reading. One explanation
is that people have radically different types of experience while reading, and individuals
or researchers tend to overgeneralize from their own case, or from one text
type or one type of reading situation, to others. Another explanation is that people are often
radically mistaken about even this seemingly obvious feature of their stream of
experience. These explanations are not incompatible, and versions of both are
endorsed by Russell T. Hurlburt, who along with his collaborators has
begun some systematic work using self-reports of sampled experience while
reading long passages of text (Brouwers
et al., 2017; Caracciolo & Hurlburt, 2016). Hurlburt and collaborators’
primary conclusions are that people vary considerably in their experience while
reading and that inner speech is much less frequent than is commonly assumed.
Hurlburt’s
work depends on his time-intensive Descriptive Experience Sampling (DES) method
(Hurlburt, 2011). DES involves extensive personal interviews about individual
moments of sampled experience, collected using a beeper. DES interviews are
conducted by expert interviewers who can, if things go well, help participants
“bracket presuppositions” in order to access their “pristine” experiences. Although
we believe DES is a valuable method, it has several shortcomings for the present
purpose: (1) Since it is time intensive, DES studies are always limited to
small samples. (2) Since DES requires expert interviewers who follow no set
script, it is difficult to replicate and it can be difficult to assess the
extent to which “experimenter effects”, such as interviewer bias, are
influencing the results. (3) To focus exclusively on the sampled experiences,
DES normally does not include other measures, such as reading comprehension
measures or other types of subjective report, which might better illuminate the
cognitive processes at issue. (For extensive discussion of the methodological
pros and cons of Descriptive Experience Sampling, see Hurlburt &
Schwitzgebel, 2007.)
Shirley
A. Long, Mark Sadoski, and their collaborators have also explored
introspectively-reported imagery experience while reading. Studying
fifth-graders’ (age 10 to 11 years) responses to poetry, narrative, and
expository writing, Long, Winograd, and Bridge (1989) found that their respondents
reported visual imagery about 60% of the time when stopped at selected points
in the passages, and that the levels of imagery reports were similar for the
different passage types. However, their power was limited by having only 26
participants in their design. Goetz, Sadoski, Stowe, and Fetsco (1993)
similarly collected introspective imagery and emotion reports from
undergraduate students while reading a full-length story. They found
correlations between the presence of reported imagery and emotion, and in a
paragraph-by-paragraph analysis, found that 7%-25% of participants reported visual
imagery for the paragraphs in question. Unfortunately, Goetz and collaborators do
not report individual differences between participants, and with only 40
participants, they had limited power to detect such differences. Furthermore,
by asking about imagery and emotion after almost every paragraph, Goetz and
collaborators may have created experimenter demand effects toward reporting
imagery after at least some paragraphs. (For similar research, see also Goetz,
Sadoski, & Olivarez, 1991; Goetz, Sadoski, Olivarez, Lee, & Roberts,
1990; Krasny & Sadoski, 2008.) Sadoski and Quast (1990) also report a
relationship between introspectively rated imagery and recall of passage
details. There is also a literature on the effect of imagery instructions in
improving reading comprehension (e.g. Cohen & Johnson, 2012; Gambrell &
Bales, 1986; Johnson, Cushman, Borden, & McCune, 2013; Sadoski, 2005) and a
literature on “narrative transportation” based on Green and Brock’s (2000)
influential measure, tending to find higher “transport” reflecting higher
levels of reader engagement and motivation. Although these research paradigms
are related to the present research question, in neither literature are
introspective reports of specific imagery experiences systematically collected.
Below
we present three studies of reading experience using medium-to-large samples of
participants and several Likert-scaled and yes/no questions about the sampled
experience. In addition to asking directly about participants’ experiences, we
also include measures intended to reveal possible differences in the cognitive
processes of readers who report different types of experience. In addition, we
varied the types of texts to which participants were exposed, which might be
expected to influence the cognitive processes employed (Kurby & Zacks,
2013; Long, Winograd, & Bridge, 1989; Nijhof & Willems, 2015) and
possibly therefore also the conscious experiences of the reader. We aimed to
test five hypotheses.
Hypothesis 1: Between subjects,
people report very different types of experience while reading. For example,
some people report frequent visual imagery while others report no visual
imagery.
Hypothesis 2: Within subjects,
people report variable types of experience while reading. For example, people
report experiencing inner speech some of the time but not all of the time while
reading.
Hypothesis 3: Different types of
texts tend to evoke reports of different types of reading experience. For
example, a passage describing rich visual detail will evoke more reports of visual
imagery than a passage of dialogue with little explicit visual detail.
Hypothesis 4: Reports of different
types of reading experience are correlated with differences in memory of the
corresponding types of textual detail. For example, people reporting visual
imagery will remember more visual detail.
Hypothesis 5: Reports of different
types of reading experience are correlated with corresponding differences in
general cognitive abilities. For example, people reporting more visual imagery
will perform better on a mental folding task.
We will
focus on experience modality, specifically
whether participants report visual imagery, inner speech, or visual experience
of the words on the page; plus self-reports of mind wandering.
2. Experiment 1
In
this experiment, we presented a poem to participants, and we asked participants
to focus on different aspects of their experience while reading the poem. We
anticipated that focusing on a particular aspect of experience while reading
might either enhance the rate at which that experience actually occurred while
reading the passage in question or – alternatively but not incompatibly –
enhance participants’ ability to detect the existence of such aspects if the
experiences are subtle or difficult to detect. When participants finished
reading, we asked them to report on their experience of the poem, and we tested
memory of specific textual details.
2.1. Methods
2.1.1.
Participants
243 participants (148 female, mean
age = 38.6, SD = 13.1) from the United States were recruited through Amazon Mechanical
Turk (MTurk) for a small fee. Although MTurk is a relatively new tool for
recruiting participants, data from MTurk appear to be as reliable as data
obtained from more traditional methods (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011;
Casler, Bickel, & Hackett, 2013; Hauser & Schwarz, 2016). Participants
were randomly assigned to one of four groups: inner speech (N = 57), visual
imagery (N = 59), words on the page (N = 51), or avoidance of mind wandering (N
= 76). All participants read the same passage and answered the same set of
questions.
2.1.2. Text
Participants read a modified version
of the poem “The Egg and the Machine” by Robert Frost (1927/1969), about 250
words long. The poem was separated into five stanzas. Two words were repeated
twice in a row (“had had” and “was was”) for use in a word recognition task.
2.1.3. Introspective Reports
Before reading the passage,
participants provided an initial set of reports about their experiences of
inner speech, visual imagery, visual experience of words on the page, and mind
wandering. Each prompt included examples to illustrate the relevant
phenomenology. For example: “How often do you experience an inner voice when
you read? Examples: you hear a voice reading in your head, you hear the
characters speaking in your head”. Participants responded using seven-point
Likert scales labeled “Never” (1), “Half of the Time” (4), and “Always” (7). Because
there is an intuitive sense in which we always experience the words on the page
when we read, we inverted the phrasing of this question, asking participants
about the absence of any perceptual experience (“How often do you NOT
experience the words on the page when you read? Examples: you’re so absorbed in
a story that it almost seems like you’re there, your mind is filled with the
ideas in the story and not the actual black letters against the white
background”). We reverse coded reports of words on the page to mirror the other
reports in the experiment. After completing the passage, participants provided
a final set of introspective reports on their experiences during the
experiment. For example: “While reading the poem, how often did you experience
inner speech? Examples: you hear a voice reading in your head, you hear the
characters speaking in your mind.” Thus, readers were asked for two different
reports, one (before reading) about their reading experiences in general, and
one (after reading) about their experience while reading the particular passage
in question. (These stimulus materials are available in the Supplementary
Online Materials, as are the raw data and all other materials used in these
three experiments.)
2.1.4. Focus Instructions
After
providing their initial introspective reports and before reading the poem,
participants in the inner speech condition were told that “Many people say they
hear inner speech when they read, such as a voice reading in their head or the
characters speaking in their mind. There is a short poem on the next page.
While reading the poem, focus on your experience of inner speech.” Participants
in the other conditions were given similar instructions: to focus on their
visual imagery, on the experience of “the actual words on the computer screen”,
or on preventing their minds from wandering.
2.1.5. Memory of Textual Details
After reading the poem, participants
answered a series of questions presented in random order. To test the
hypothesis that participants reporting greater amounts of inner speech would
find rhyme more salient and memorable, we asked participants to identify 10
rhyming pairs from the poem out of a list of 20. To test the hypothesis that
reporting greater visual experience of the words on the page would influence
memory of visual details of the textual presentation, we asked participants to
identify the poem’s font from a list of four dissimilar fonts, to identify the
two words that were repeated twice (e.g. “had had”) out of a list of nine, to remember
whether 10 two-word phrases appeared at the beginning or end of line, and to remember
whether 10 lines appeared at the beginning or end of a stanza. Because the
behavioral measure involved short-term memory, participants also performed a
memory task for 3-digit numbers. Finally, a reading comprehension question
tested for basic understanding of the text. (Participants also performed a
Stroop task. However, reaction time data for the Stroop task were not properly
recorded and so those data are excluded from analysis.)
2.1.6. Procedure
Participants took part in the
experiment on their own computers using a web browser and the Lime Survey platform.
We collected demographic information and then solicited the initial set of
introspective reports. We then randomly sorted participants into the four
conditions. Reading the poem took about 1-2 minutes. After reading the poem,
participants provided the final set of introspective reports, then responded to
the behavioral measures. The experiment took about 15 minutes to complete.
2.2.
Results
After
excluding responses from 15 participants who answered the comprehension
question incorrectly, we included 228 participants in the analysis. Initial
introspective reports were intended primarily as training and correlated
moderately well with the final introspective reports (r = .33 to .55). Figure 1
shows the distribution of the final set of introspective reports. Reports of
experience differed substantially between participants, with participants
spread widely across most of the 1-7 range for all three modalities.
Participants also reported variable experience, with only a minority of
participants reporting “always” or “never” experiencing visual imagery, inner
speech, or the words on the page. Visual imagery was most commonly reported (M
= 4.8, SD = 1.6), followed by words on the page (M = 4.6, SD = 1.6), inner
speech (M = 4.3, SD = 2.0), and mind wandering (M = 2.9, SD = 1.6). Except for
reports of visual imagery and words on the page, all differences between means were
pairwise significant (paired t ≥ 2.2, p < .03). Contrary to our
expectations, the focus instructions did not have any statistically detectable
influence on the final introspective reports (ANOVAs [3, 224], F ≤ 1.4, p
≥ .26).
Figure 1
Histogram
of retrospective introspective reports of reading experience provided after
reading a poem.
Participants
who reported more inner speech experience did not perform detectably better on
the questions targeting memory of phonological aspects of the text such as
rhyming pairs (r = .11, p = .11), nor did participants reporting more experience
of the words on the page perform detectably better on the questions targeting
memory of visual aspects of the text such as font type (r = -.01, p = .94). G*Power
analysis found that this study had sufficient sensitivity to detect a
medium-sized effect (r = .18) at the .05 alpha level with a power of .80.
Reports
of mind wandering correlated negatively with reports of visual imagery (r =
-.33, p < .001), reports of the words on the page (r = -.22, p = .001), and with
our general test of memory performance (r = -.15, p = .03). We interpret a high
degree of reported mind wandering as indicating low participant engagement.
2.3. Discussion
Confirming
Hypotheses 1 and 2, participants reported very different experiences while
reading, and most reported variable experience in the course of reading. For
example, few participants reported “always” experiencing inner speech while
reading, despite the widespread view among philosophers and psychologists that
reading typically involves consciously experienced inner speech. The failure of
the focus instructions to have any detectable effect on the reported modality
of experiences might have been due to (1) participants failing to comply with
the focus instruction over the 1-2 minutes of reading, (2) a weak relationship
between focus on one modality and experience in that same modality, (3) failure
of the retrospective reports to capture participants’ actual experience, or (4)
limited statistical power. The failure to detect a relationship between
reported modality of experience and memory of corresponding features of the
text might be due to either (1) failures of retrospective report, (2) limited
statistical power, or (3) performance on the memory questions depending on
cognitive factors unrelated to the factors influencing the subjective reports.
3. Experiment 2
For
Experiment 2, we increased the number of participants to improve statistical
power, interrupted the reading experience with a beep to solicit a more
immediate report of a single sampled moment of experience, and changed the
measures of participants’ memory of textual detail. We also wanted to explore
the effects of passage type on modal experience (Hypothesis 3). Participants
were either shown a poem with vivid rhythm and rhyme, a dialogue from a play
written for the theater, or a prose passage full of dramatic dialogue or rich
visual detail. Participants who read a poem were subsequently asked a question
based on rhyme. We hypothesized that participants with abundant inner speech
would be more likely to answer that question correctly. Participants given the
dialogue or prose passages were either asked about the colors of objects in the
story or about details of the visual presentation of the text on the screen. We
hypothesized that participants reporting abundant visual imagery would more
accurately remember color and that participants reporting frequently
experiencing the words on the page would more accurately remember details of
the visual presentation of the text.
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
We
recruited 1,457 participants (800 female, mean age = 25.7, SD = 10.9), 864 from
the Psychology Subject Pool at University of California, Riverside, and 593 who
received a small payment through Amazon MTurk. All MTurk participants were from
the United States and reported English as their first language. Participants
were randomly sorted into one of three conditions: inner speech, visual
imagery, or words on the page.
3.1.2. Texts
The texts were four tightly rhymed
poems, one stage dialogue, one prose passage with dramatic dialogue, and two
visually vivid descriptive passages, each with minor variations. Each poem
contained one novel word, used twice (e.g., the name “Tennaise”), the
pronunciation of which was disambiguated by the rhyme scheme (e.g., “And their
gallant moves precise / Sailing safely into port / Chased by beautiful
Tenaisse?”). The dialogue and descriptive passages differed in the color terms
used to describe central objects (e.g., “tan” vs “golden” leaves) and in the
font and page format in which they were presented. There were two versions of
each poem and four versions of each of the remaining passages, for a total of
24 unique passages. Participants in the inner speech condition were always
assigned a poem. Participants in the other two conditions were assigned one of
the other passages. All passages were approximately 500 words long.
3.1.3. Introspective Reports
In
addition to soliciting general introspective reports before and after reading,
as in Experiment 1, this study solicited concrete reports of experience at a
specific moment in time. Participants heard a one-second 500 Hz beep through
their computer speakers at a random time 30-90 seconds after the page loaded. After
the beep, a new page loaded automatically and asked participants to report
their experience “in the final split second before the beep”. Participants
responded “Yes”, “No”, or “Maybe / Don’t Know” for inner speech, visual
imagery, words on the page, and mind wandering. Thus, we collected three types
of introspective reports: general reports about reading experience (collected
before reading the assigned passage), a concrete “beeped” report about a
particular moment of experience (collected immediately upon interrupting
reading with a beep), and a general retrospective report about these experience
while reading the passage (collected after having completed the entirety of the
passage). We also asked participants to describe their experience in a free-response
text box. We used these responses to exclude concrete reports from participants
who reported that they had already finished reading at the time of the beep. We
also intend to analyze these free responses as an exploratory basis for future
research, but we will not further report on them in this article.
3.1.4. Memory of Textual Details
After reading the passage,
participants in all three conditions answered two reading comprehension
questions, and either a phonological question, questions about visual details
of the story, or questions about visual details of the text on the screen.
Phonological
question. Participants
in the inner speech condition were asked to identify a word that rhymed with
the target novel word. Pronunciation of the novel word had been disambiguated
by the rhyme scheme, and the word was used twice. For example, “Tenaisse” had
been disambiguated by “precise”, and the memory question asked whether, in the
context of the poem, the word “Tenaisse” rhymed with “vice” [correct], “ace”,
“lacy”, or “spicy”.
Questions
about visual details of the story.
Participants in the visual imagery condition were asked about the color of an
object prominent in the visual scene but whose color was not relevant to the action.
The object was always described using a moderate frequency color term, while
the memory question employed a basic color term in the same semantic space (for
example “golden” leaves in the text and participants were asked if the leaves
were “yellow” [correct], “green”, “brown”, or “red”).
Questions
about visual presentation of the words on the screen. Participants in the words-on-the-page
condition were asked to identify the font in which the passage was presented
and the quadrant in which a particular phrase had appeared in a two-column,
multi-page text.
3.1.5. Procedure
Participants took part in the
experiment on their own computers using a web browser and the Lime Survey
platform. After a sound check, participants provided general reports on their experience
while reading and then read one of the 24 passages, determined at random. While
reading the passage, participants heard a beep at a random time in the first
30-90 seconds. This prompted a new page to load, instructing participants to
provide concrete reports on their experiences. Participants then returned to
the passage and finished reading. Afterwards, participants answered two
comprehension questions and 1-3 memory questions depending on condition. The
experiment concluded with a final set of general introspective reports. The
entire study took about 15 minutes.
3.2. Results
We
excluded 224 participants who answered the comprehension questions incorrectly,
spent less than ten seconds reading the passage after giving the concrete
report, or who indicated in their response box that they had already completed
the passage at the time of the beep, leaving 1,233 participants for analysis.
Exclusion rates were somewhat higher in the visually vivid descriptive passage
condition (98/444 [22%]) than in the dialogue (56/507 [11%]) and poetry (70/506
[14%]) conditions. Figure 2 shows the distribution of the final set of general
reports. The data are similar to Experiment 1, with visual imagery reported
most often (M = 5.0, SD = 1.5), followed by inner speech (M = 4.6, SD = 1.9),
words on the page (M = 4.3, SD = 1.6), then mind wandering (M = 3.5, SD = 1.7),
with all differences between means pairwise significant (t ≥ 4.9, p <
.001). Figure 3 shows the distribution of concrete reports. Compatible with the
general report means of somewhat over 4 (“Half of the Time”), participants
reported each of the types of modal experiences in somewhat more than half of
the sampled moments, again with visual imagery the most frequent (visual
imagery 70%, inner speech 59%, and words on the page 56%; χ2
[4] = 76.9, p < .001). Participants reported a unimodal experience in 23% of
concrete reports (e.g. inner speech with no visual imagery or experience of the
words on the page), while 66% reported a multi-modal experience, with 11%
reporting no modal experience whatsoever. The concrete reports of mind
wandering in 29% of probes are broadly consistent with Schooler et al. (2004),
who found participants “caught” mind wandering while reading in 23% of probes.
Figure 2
Histogram
of retrospective introspective reports of reading experience across all
conditions and passage types in Experiment 2.
Figure 3
Histogram
of the concrete “beeped” introspective reports of reading experience across all
conditions and passage types in Experiment 2.
Participants
reported more inner speech in the dramatic dialogue passages than in the
passages emphasizing visual description, and conversely more visual imagery in
the descriptive passages than in the dramatic dialogue passages, though the
effect sizes were small to moderate. The poetry passages had both phonological
and imagistic elements and tended to be intermediate. See Table 1 for details.
Table 1
Difference
in reported visual imagery and inner speech by passage type across all
conditions.
Passage
type |
Descriptive |
Poetry |
Dialogue |
Test
Statistic |
Mean
visual imagerya |
5.2 |
4.9 |
5.0 |
F(2,1230)
= 4.3, p = .01, η2 = .007 |
%
visual imageryb |
78% |
66% |
69%
|
χ2(4)
= 14.4, p = .006, Cramer’s V=.076 |
Mean
inner speecha |
4.3 |
4.6 |
4.9 |
F(2,1230)
= 11.2, p < .001, η2 = .018 |
%
inner speechb |
53% |
59% |
65% |
χ2(4)
= 19.1, p = .001, Cramer’s V=.088 |
aThe mean general report
of the experience of visual imagery or inner speech. bPercentage of
affirmative concrete “beeped” reports.
Differences
in reported experience did not significantly correlate with memory for
corresponding passage details, although there were two marginally significant
trends with small effect sizes in the predicted direction. See Table 2. G*Power
analyses show that this study had sufficient sensitivity to detect weak
correlations (r = .13-.14, depending on condition) at the .05 alpha level with
a power of .80.
Table 2
Two-tailed
Pearson correlations between introspective reports and memory of textual detail
across all conditions.
Behavioral
Measures |
Introspective
Reports |
Correlation (p) |
N |
|
Visual Story Details |
Visual Imagery |
General Concrete |
.09 (.07) .01 (.91) |
409 |
Phonological Details |
Inner Speech |
General Concrete |
.04 (.44) .08 (.08) |
436 |
Details of Visual Presentation |
Words on the Page |
General Concrete |
.00 (.94) -.02 (.67) |
388 |
General
reports of mind wandering correlated negatively with reports of modal
experience (visual imagery: r = -.29, p < .001; inner speech: r = -.11, p
< .001; words on the page: r = -.17, p < .001) and also correlated
negatively with our measures of memory performance (visual story detail: r =
-.14, p = .005; phonological questions: r = -.12, p = .01; details of visual
presentation: r = -.10, p = .04). This is consistent with the results from
Experiment 1.
3.3. Discussion
Confirming both Hypotheses 1 and 2,
there was substantial variability in reported modal experiences, both between
and within subjects. Extending the results of Experiment 1, and in accord with
Hypothesis 3, we confirmed that different types of text elicited different
types of introspective report. Participants reported the most visual imagery
when they read texts with vivid descriptions of visual detail, and they
reported the most inner speech when they read dramatic dialogues. Despite
sufficient statistical power to detect even fairly small correlations, we found
no relationship between reported modality of experience and performance on
seemingly-related memory tasks, contrary to Hypothesis 4. However, there were
in some cases statically marginal trends in the predicted directions.
4. Experiment 3
Individual
differences in types of reading experience might correlate with differences in
general cognitive abilities (Hypothesis 5). Experiment 3 was aimed at testing
this hypothesis, using a “mental folding” task to test visual imagery ability
and a phonological interference task to test reliance on phonological
information in reading. If introspective reports of inner speech reflect
reliance on phonological processing while reading, then participants who report
high levels of conscious inner speech might perform worse on a word memory test
designed to be difficult due to the phonological similarity of the target words.
We
also wanted to see if a more powerful study could reveal a relationship between
reported modality of experience and memory for corresponding details. Since the
visual story detail task came closest to statistical significance in Experiment
2, in Experiment 3 we improved power by presenting visual story detail
questions to all participants. We also changed the final set of introspective
reports so that they matched the introspective reports used at the beginning of
the study. That is, we asked participants to assess what is generally true of their reading
experience both before and after the study, so that we could examine the
reliability of participants’ answers to this type of question.
4.1. Methods
4.1.1. Participants
595 participants (291 female, mean age = 34.2, SD =
11.2) were recruited through Amazon MTurk for a small fee. All participants
were from the United States and reported English as their first language. Participants
were randomly assigned one of three passages to read.
4.1.2. Texts
The passages used in this study were
slightly modified versions of the three prose passages used in Experiment 2. All
were presented in a single page of text.
4.1.3. Introspective Reports
This experiment recorded general
and concrete reports using the same method as in Experiment 2, with two
exceptions. First, because Experiments 1 and 2 did not find even a marginally
significant correlation between reports of experiencing the words on the page and
memory of visual details of textual presentation of the words on the page, we
did not ask for this type of report. Second, at the end of the previous experiments
we asked participants to report retrospectively on their experience “while
reading the passage”, however in this study we asked participants exactly the
same introspective questions at the end as at the beginning (e.g., about how
often they experience visual imagery when they read). We prefaced these final
introspective reports by saying “In light of this experiment, you may have
changed your opinions about your experiences while reading. It’s fine if they
changed or stayed the same, just do your best to answer truthfully”.
4.1.4. Behavioral Measures
Participants answered two general
comprehension questions in addition to two questions about visual story
details, similarly to Experiment 2. The folding task consisted of 10 questions
that tested the ability to mentally fold a piece of paper, taken from Ekstrom
et al. (1976). For the phonological interference task, participants were shown
five words in a series for one second each, drawn at random from a list of ten
words and were asked to recognize the list in order from the entire set of ten.
After a training set that was not used in the analysis (old, deep, foul, late,
safe, great, strong, thin, long, broad), participants performed the task six
times, switching off between a phonologically similar set (mad, man, map, mat,
max, can, cad, cap, cat, cab) and a control set of phonologically different
words (pen, rig, day, bar, cow, sup, pit, hot, few, bun). The design and word
sets come from the first experiment in Baddeley (1966).
4.1.5. Procedure
The procedure was the same as
Experiment 2 through the end of the reading passage. After completing the
passage, participants answered two comprehension question and two questions
about visual story details, presented in random order, followed by a final set
of general introspective reports. Finally, participants performed the mental
folding task and the phonological interference task, again presented in random
order. The experiment took about 20 minutes to complete.
4.2. Results
Using
the same criteria as the previous study, we excluded responses from 48
participants, leaving 547 participants in the analysis. When probed with a
beep, 82% of participants reported visual imagery, 57% reported inner speech,
and 15% reported mind wandering (χ2 [4] = 562, p < .001). In
the final general reports, visual imagery (M = 5.4, SD = 1.4) was reported more
frequently than inner speech (M = 4.9, SD = 1.7) and mind wandering (M = 3.1,
SD = 1.4), with all differences pairwise significant (paired t ≥ 6.0, p
< .001). These results correspond to the results in Experiments 1 and 2 but
are not strictly comparable because the final reports in Experiment 3 concern
reading experience in general while the final reports in Experiments 1 and 2
concern experience while reading the presented passage. Participants reported a
unimodal experience in 42% of concrete reports (e.g. inner speech with no
visual imagery), while 49% reported a multi-modal experience, with 9% reporting
no modal experience whatsoever.
Table
3 shows the results of two-tailed Pearson correlations between the three
introspective reports recorded in this experiment. Because we solicited the
same general reports at the beginning and end of the experiment, this served as
a measure of test-retest reliability. We believe that these correlations (r =
.70 to .87) fall within acceptable range. Since the intervening time between
reports was less than 15 minutes, a longer test-retest delay might show
substantially lower correlations. We did not find that participants who
reported concrete experience in one modality tended to shift their final
general reports to better match their concrete reports. We measured this by comparing
their concrete reports in each modality (coding them as -1 for no, 0 for don’t
know, and +1 for yes) with the difference between their final general report
(on the 1-7 scale) and their initial general report (on the same scale, so that
for example a shift from 1 “Never” to 4 “Half of the Time” would be +3). If the
concrete experience report influences the final general report, then the
concrete reports should correlate positively with the difference between the
final general reports and the initial general reports. However, we did not find
a statistically significant correlation for either modality (inner speech r =
.03, p = 43; visual imagery r = -.06, p = .15).
Table 3
Pearson
correlations between introspective reports in Experiment 3.
|
|
Initial
General Reports |
Final
General Reports |
Concrete
Reports |
||||||
|
|
Inner Speech |
Visual Imagery |
Mind Wander |
Inner Speech |
Visual Imagery |
Mind Wander |
Inner Speech |
Visual Imagery |
Mind Wander |
Initial General Reports |
Inner
Speech |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Visual
Imagery |
.46 (<.001) |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Mind
Wander |
-.06 (.20) |
-.03 (.54) |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Final General Reports |
Inner
Speech |
.87 (<.001) |
.46 (<.001) |
-.05 (.30) |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
Visual
Imagery |
.43 (<.001) |
.77 (<.001) |
-.05 (.22) |
.43 (<.001) |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
Mind
Wander |
.01 (.83) |
-.02 (.66) |
.70 (<.001) |
.00 (.94) |
.02 (.61) |
1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Concrete Reports |
Inner
Speech |
.55 (<.001) |
.13 (.002) |
-.08 (.05) |
.57 (<.001) |
.18 (<.001) |
-.05 (.23) |
1 |
|
|
Visual
Imagery |
.11 (.01) |
.40 (<.001) |
-.13 (.003) |
.17 (<.001) |
.36 (<.001) |
-.12 (.007) |
.10 (.02) |
1 |
|
|
Mind
Wander |
-.03 (.46) |
-.04 (.38) |
.31 (<.001) |
-.09 (.03) |
-.03 (.42) |
.31 (<.001) |
-.14 (.001) |
-.30 (<.001) |
1 |
Note. Statistical
significance of the correlation is in parentheses. Corresponding modal reports
are in bold. N = 547.
Table
4 shows the results from a planned series of two-tailed Pearson correlations
between introspective reports and behavioral measures. Because this experiment
did not include multiple conditions, statistical power was higher than in the
previous study despite the lower total number of participants. While Experiment
2 found a marginally significant correlation between memory of visual story
detail and general reports of visual
imagery, this study found a significant correlation between memory of visual
detail and concrete reports of imagery.
Contrary to Hypothesis 5, we did not find significant relationships in the
predicted directions between introspective reports and either the folding task
or the phonological interference task. In fact, we found a small but
statistically significant correlation in the unexpected direction between mental folding and reported visual
imagery, a result for which we have no interpretation. This study had
sufficient sensitivity to detect weak correlations (r = .12) at the .05 alpha
level with a power of .80.
Table 4
Two-tailed
Pearson correlations between introspective reports and behavioral measures in
Experiment 3.
Behavioral
Measures |
Introspective
Reports |
Correlation (p) |
|
||
Visual Detail Questions |
Visual Imagery |
General Concrete |
.07 (.11) .10 (.03) |
||
Folding Task |
Visual Imagery |
General Concrete |
-.09 (.04) .05 (.24) |
||
Phonological Similarity |
Inner Speech |
General Concrete |
-.03 (.53) -.01 (.92) |
||
4.3. Discussion
Experiment 3 confirms the high levels of variability
in introspective report between and within subjects, as also found in
Experiments 1 and 2. We also found reasonable test-retest reliability for our
general introspective measures, and we found evidence in support of a weak correlation
between reported visual imagery experience and memory for visual story detail. However,
we found no evidence of a relationship between introspective reports and tests
of mental folding ability or susceptibility to phonological interference.
5. General Discussion
Across
all three experiments we found consistent evidence supporting four broad
conclusions:
Confirming
Hypothesis 1, between subjects, people report very different types of
experience while reading. Some people report always experiencing visual imagery
while reading, while others report never doing so. Some people report always
experiencing inner speech while reading, while others report never doing so. Some
people report always experiencing the words on the page, while others report
never doing so. Similarly, participants differed in their concrete reports of
single individually sampled experiences.
Confirming
Hypothesis 2, within subjects, people report variable types of experience while
reading. The majority of participants avoided the end points of the Likert
scale for each of the three types of modal experience. That is, only a minority
of participants reported either “always” or “never” experiencing visual
imagery, inner speech, or the words on the page while reading.
Confirming
Hypothesis 3, in Experiment 2 we found evidence that text type influenced
reported reading experience. Inner speech was more commonly reported in
dramatic dialogues than in passages with vivid visual detail, and visual
imagery was more commonly reported in passages with vivid visual detail than in
dramatic dialogues.
Finally,
although we did not anticipate this result, in all three experiments, visual
imagery was reported more commonly than was inner speech for most of the types
of texts we used. Even in dramatic dialogue passages, inner speech was only
moderately more frequently reported than visual imagery.
We
interpret these four results as broadly confirming findings by Hurlburt and
colleagues, who report highly variable reading experience both within and
between subjects and whose participants report inner speech in only a minority
of samples (Brouwers et al., 2017; Caracciolo & Hurlburt, 2016). However, our methodology is more
readily replicable than Hurlburt’s and we have many more participants. The
result regarding the prevalence of inner speech is especially important due to
the number of researchers who assume that inner speech is normally present as
part of the conscious phenomenology of reading experience (e.g., Baars, 2003; Filik
& Barber, 2011; Kivy, 2008; Morin, 2009; Rayner, et al., 2012; Velmans,
2009). Brouwers and colleagues, in discussing some of our preliminary data (available
in Moore, 2016; Moore & Schwitzgebel, 2013), raise the possibility that our
participants overreport inner speech due to failing to sufficiently “bracket
the presupposition” that inner speech must be present. We do not necessarily
reject this possibility.
The
validity of these measures of reading experience is partly confirmed. In
Experiment 3, we found evidence of test-retest reliability over an interval of
15 minutes. In Experiments 2 and 3, we also found the expected match between
retrospective general reports and the percentages of reported modal experience
in individually sampled (beeped) concrete moments of experience. For example,
participants’ mean retrospective response to how often they experienced visual
imagery in Experiment 2 was 5.0 on a scale where 4 was marked “Half of the Time”
and 7 was marked “Always”. Broadly in accord with that result, participants
reported visual imagery in 70% of sampled moments.
Contrary
to Hypothesis 4, we found little evidence of a substantial relationship between
modal introspective reports (visual imagery, inner speech, or visual experience
of the words on the page) and memory of seeming-related textual details (of
visual story detail, of phonological information such as the pronunciation of
words disambiguated by rhyme scheme, or of details of the visual presentation
of the text such as font). Despite sufficient power to detect small-to-medium
effect sizes, across the three experiments we found only one statistically
significant association in the predicted direction (concrete “beeped” reports
of visual imagery and memory of visual story detail in Experiment 3). The
relationship between reported conscious experience while reading and memory of
seemingly-related textual detail appears small to nonexistent.
Contrary
to Hypothesis 5, in Experiment 3 we found no evidence of a positive
relationship between reports of visual imagery and performance on a mental
folding task, and we found no evidence of a relationship between reports of
inner speech and phonological interference in a later memory task.
This
lack of relationship is not entirely surprising. As reviewed in Schwitzgebel
(2011), there have generally been weak and unsystematic relationships between
subjective measures of visual imagery such as the VVIQ (Marks, 1979), and
performance on cognitive tasks widely thought to be facilitated by visual
imagery. Similarly, despite continuing popularity in some educational circles,
there is little evidence of a relationship between students’ preferred
“learning styles” (e.g., visual vs. auditory) and educational performance (Felder
& Spurlin, 2005; Pashler et al., 2008). Our results fit this general
pattern.
We
see two broad interpretative possibilities. One possibility, defended at length
in Schwitzgebel (2011), is that introspective reports of this sort do not
accurately reflect people’s actual streams of conscious experience. People are
simply bad introspectors. Another possibility, advocated for example by Paivio (1986),
is that conscious experiences of this sort are being accurately reported by
participants but have little relationship to basic cognitive functions such as
memory, mental rotation, or phonological processing.
Reader
response theory and some other approaches to literary criticism and aesthetics
emphasize the importance of the reader’s experience while encountering a text (Fish,
1970; Holland, 1975; Iser, 1976/1978; Kivy, 2008; Robinson, 2005; Rosenblatt, 1978).
Systematic experience sampling during aesthetic experiences such as reading,
watching films, listening to music, or witnessing live performances on stage
has great potential as a research methodology in empirical aesthetics,
especially if the validity of the reports can be confirmed. We predict that
experience sampling will soon become a common method for studying these types
of phenomena. We hope that the present research lays some of the groundwork for
this nascent field of study.
Acknowledgements.
This work
was supported by an Academic Senate grant from the University of California at
Riverside.
References
1. Ahsen, A. (1984). Reading of image in psychology and
literary text. Journal of Mental Imagery,
8(3), 1-32.
2. Baars, B. J. (2003). How brain reveals mind. Journal
of Consciousness Studies, 10(9-10). 100-114.
3. Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The influence of acoustic
and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 18(4), 302-309.
4.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic
buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(11),
417-423.
5.
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working
memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press.
6.
Baddeley, A. D. (2010). Working
memory. Current Biology, 20(4), R136-R140.
7. Berkeley, G. (1710/1965). A treatise concerning the
principles of human knowledge. In C. Turbayne (Ed.), Principles, Dialogues,
and Philosophical Correspondence. Macmillan.
8.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., &
Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive,
yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1),
3-5.
9.
Burke, E.
(1757/1990). A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the
sublime and beautiful. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
10. Brouwers,
V. P., Heavey, C. L., Lapping-Carr, L., Moynihan, S., Kelsey, J., &
Hurlburt, R. T. (2017). Silent reading is
not silent speaking of the text: An investigation of pristine inner experience
while reading. Manuscript.
11. Caracciolo,
M., & Hurlburt, R. T. (2016). A
passion for specificity. The Ohio State University Press.
12. Carroll,
N. (2001). Beyond aesthetics: Philosophical essays. Cambridge
University Press.
13. Casler,
K., Bickel, L., & Hackett, E. (2013). Separate but equal? A comparison of
participants and data gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and
face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6),
2156-2160.
14. Cohen,
M. T., & Johnson, H. L. (2011). Improving the acquisition of novel
vocabulary through the use of imagery interventions. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 38(5), 357-366.
15. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., &
Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition
and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.
16. Dennett,
D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. USA: Back Bay Books.
17. Ekstrom,
R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, W. (1976). Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests.
Educational Testing Service.
18. Faw, B.
(2009). Conflicting intuitions may be based on different abilities: Evidence
from mental imaging research. Journal of
Consciousness Studies, 16(4), 45-68.
19. Felder,
R. M., & Spurlin, J. (2005). Applications, reliability, and validity of the
index of learning styles. International
Journal of Engineering Education, 21(1), 103-112.
20. Filik,
R., & Barber, E. (2011). Inner speech during silent reading reflects the
reader's regional accent. PloS one, 6(10).
21. Fish, S.
(1970). Literature in the reader: Affective stylistics. New Literary History, 2(1),
123-162.
22. Frost, R.
L. (1927/1969). The egg and the machine. In The poetry of Robert Frost
(269). Henry Holt and Company.
23. Frost, R.
(1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True
issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 71.
24. Gambrell,
L. B., & Bales, R. J. (1986). Mental imagery and the
comprehension-monitoring performance of fourth-and fifth-grade poor
readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 454-464.
25. Goetz, E.
T., Sadoski, M., & Olivarez, Jr., A. (1991). Getting a reading on reader
response: Relationships between imagery, affect, and importance ratings,
recall, and imagery reports. Reading Psychology: An International
Quarterly, 12(1), 13-26.
26. Goetz, E.
T., Sadoski, M., Stowe, M. L., Fetsco, T. G., & Kemp, S. G. (1993). Imagery
and emotional response in reading literary text: Quantitative and qualitative
analyses. Poetics, 22(1-2), 35-49.
27. Green, M.
C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness
of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5),
701.
28. Hauser,
D. J., & Schwarz, N. (2016). Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants perform
better on online attention checks than do subject pool participants. Behavior
Research Methods, 48(1),
400-407.
29. Holland,
N. (1975). 5 readers reading. Yale
University Press.
30. Hurlburt,
R. (2011). Investigating pristine inner
experience: Moments of truth. Cambridge University Press.
31. Hurlburt, R.T. & E. Schwitzgebel (2007). Describing
inner experience: Proponent meets skeptic. MIT Press.
32. Iser,
W. (1976/1978). The act of reading: A theory of aesthetic
response. Johns Hopkins University Press.
33. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology.
New York: Henry Holt.
34. Jaynes, J. (1976/2000). The origin of
consciousness and the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
35. Johnson, D. R., Cushman, G. K., Borden, L. A., &
McCune, M. S. (2013). Potentiating empathic growth: Generating imagery while
reading fiction increases empathy and prosocial behavior. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 306.
36. Kivy,
P. (2008). The performance of reading: An essay in the philosophy of
literature. John Wiley & Sons.
37. Krasny,
K. A., & Sadoski, M. (2008). Mental imagery and affect in English/French
bilingual readers: A cross-linguistic perspective. Canadian Modern
Language Review, 64(3), 399-428.
38. Kurby,
C. A., Magliano, J. P., & Rapp, D. N. (2009). Those voices in your head:
Activation of auditory images during reading. Cognition, 112, 457–461.
39. Kurby,
C. A. & Zacks, J. M. (2013). The activation of modality-specific
representations during discourse processing. Brain and Language, 126(3), 338-349.
40. Lamarque,
P., & Olsen, S. H. (2004). The philosophy of literature: Pleasure
restored. The Blackwell guide to aesthetics, 195-214.
41. Lauro,
L. J. R., Reis, J., Cohen, L. G., Cecchetto, C., & Papagno, C. (2010). A
case for the involvement of phonological loop in sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 48(14),
4003-4011.
42. Leinenger,
M. (2014). Phonological coding during reading. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1534.
43. Long,
S. A., Winograd, P. N., & Bridge, C. A. (1989). The effects of reader and
text characteristics on imagery reported during and after reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 353-372.
44. Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in
the recall of pictures. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 17-24.
45. Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (2002). A feeling for
fiction: Becoming what we behold. Poetics, 30(4),
221-241.
46. Moore, A.T., (2016). The experience of reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Department of Philosophy, University of California at Riverside.
47. Moore, A.T., & Schwitzgebel, E. (2013). The
experience of reading: Imagery, inner speech, and seeing the words on the page.
Blog post at The Splintered Mind (Aug
28, 2013). URL:
http://schwitzsplinters.blogspot.com/2013/08/the-experience-of-reading-imagery-inner.html
48. Morin, A. (2009). Inner speech and consciousness. In
W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consciousness, vol. 1 (389-402).
Oxford: Elsevier.
49. Nannicelli,
T. (2013). A philosophy of the screenplay. Routledge.
50. Nijhof, A. D., & Willems, R. M. (2015).
Simulating fiction: Individual differences in literature comprehension revealed
with fMRI. PLoS One, 10(2): e0116492.
51. Oppenheim,
G. M., & Dell, G. S. (2008). Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but
not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition, 106(1),
528-537.
52. Paap,
K. R., & Noel, R. W. (1991). Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a
good horse race. Psychological Research, 53(1), 13-24.
53. Pashler, H., McDaniel, M., Rohrer, D., & Bjork,
R. (2008). Learning styles concepts and evidence. Psychological Science in
the Public Interest, 9(3), 105-119.
54. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford. England:
Oxford University Press.
55. Perrone-Bertolotti,
M., Rapin, L., Lachaux, J. P., Baciu, M., & Loevenbruck, H. (2014). What is
that little voice inside my head? Inner speech phenomenology, its role in
cognitive performance, and its relation to self-monitoring. Behavioural
Brain Research, 261,
220-239.
56. Pham,
A. V., & Hasson, R. M. (2014). Verbal and visuospatial working memory as
predictors of children's reading ability. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 29(5), 467-477.
57. Phelan,
J. (2007). Experiencing fiction: Judgments, progressions, and the
rhetorical theory of narrative. The Ohio State University Press.
58. Rayner,
K., Pollatsek, A., Ashby, J., & Clifton Jr, C. (2012). Psychology
of reading. Psychology Press.
59. Reed,
H. B. (1916). The existence and function of inner speech in thought processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 365-392.
60. Robinson,
J. (2005). Deeper than reason: Emotion and its role in literature,
music, and art. Oxford University Press on Demand.
61. Rosenblatt,
L. M. (1978). The reader, the text, the poem: The transactional theory
of the literary work. Southern Illinois University Press.
62. Sadoski,
M. (2005). A dual coding view of vocabulary learning. Reading &
Writing Quarterly, 21(3), 221-238.
63. Sadoski,
M., Goetz, E. T., Olivarez Jr, A., Lee, S., & Roberts, N. M. (1990).
Imagination in story reading: The role of imagery, verbal recall, story
analysis, and processing levels. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22(1),
55-70.
64. Sadoski,
M., & Quast, Z. (1990). Reader response and long-term recall for
journalistic text: The roles of imagery, affect, and importance. Reading
Research Quarterly, 256-272.
65. Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D.
V. (2004). Zoning out while reading: Evidence for dissociations between
experience and metaconsciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing:
Visual metacognition in adults and children (203-226). Massachusetts
Institute of Technology.
66. Schwitzgebel, E. (2011). Perplexities of
consciousness. MIT press.
67. Seidenberg,
M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of
word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4),
523.
68. Siewert, C. (1998). The significance of
consciousness. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
69. Van
Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in
reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3),
371.
70. Vellmans,
M. (2009). Understanding consciousness, 2nd edition. Routledge.
71. Wittgenstein,
L. (1946-48/1975). Zettel. Translated by G. E. M. Anscombe. Edited by G.
E. M. Anscombe and G. H. von Wright. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of
California Press.
72. Woodworth, R. S. (1906). Imageless thought. The
Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 3(26),
701-708.
73. Zwaan,
R. A. (2016). Situation models, mental simulations, and abstract concepts in
discourse comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1028-1034.
74. Zwaan,
R. A. & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension
and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162.
[1] Corresponding author: Alan Tonnies Moore, Philosophy Department, Humanities 388, 1600 Holloway Avenue, San Francisco, CA 94132. Email: alantonniesmoore@gmail.com.